•  
  •  
 

Keywords

inferential theory, direct theory, visual cognition, perception and action, conceptualization, propositional knowledge

Abstract

Two contrasting theories, or variants of them, are predominant in the current debate on visual cognition. The standard inferential theory sees perception as a process involving the role of memory, past experiences and semantic abilities, whereas the direct theory sees perception as a connection between the perceiver and the environment that does not recruit internal information processing. In particular, the direct theory has recently been invoked because it would be able to explain the sensorimotor coupling of perception and action in humans and animals without relying on controversial notions such as those of conceptualization and propositional information. This paper aims to show that even an inferential theory of perception has enough resources to account for sensorimotor processes without necessarily involving high level cognitive functions. My claim is that there are genuine instances of sensorimotor inferential processing that do not rely on conceptual structures and propositional knowledge. Several theoretical and empirical arguments are provided to support this statement.

Creative Commons License

Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-No Derivative Works 4.0 License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-No Derivative Works 4.0 License.

References

Ashby, F. G., Prinzmetal, W., Ivry, R., Maddox, T. 1996. “A formal theory of feature binding in object perception”, Psychological Review, 103, 1, 165-192.

Barsalou, L. W. 2005. Situated Conceptualization. In Cohen, H., Lefevfre, C., Handbook of Categorization in Cognitive Science, Elsevier.

Biederman, I. 1995. Visual object recognition. In Osherson, D. ed. An invitation to cognitive science, Vol. 2 Visual cognition, MIT Press, 121-165.

Borra, E., Belmalih, A., Calzavara, R., Gerbella, M., Murata, A., Rozzi, S., Luppino, G. 2008. “Cortical Connections of the Macaque Anterior Intraparietal AIP Area”, Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1094-1111.

Buccino, G. S., Cattaneo, L., Rodà, F., & Riggio, L. 2009. “Broken affordances, broken objects: A TMS study”, Neuropsychologia, 47, 3074–3078.

Butterfill, S., Sinigaglia, C. 2012 “Intention and motor representation in purposive action”, Philosophy and Phenomenological Research doi: 10.1111/j.1933-1592.2012.00604.x

Campion, J., Latto, R. 1985 “Apperceptive agnosia due to carbon monoxide poisoning. An interpretation based on critical band masking from disseminated lesions” Behavioral and Brain Research,15, 227-240.

Caramazza, A., Shelton, J. R. 1998. “Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain: The animate-inanimate distinction”, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10,1-34.

Cardellicchio, P., Sinigaglia, C., & Costantini, M. 2011. “The space of affordances: A TMS study”, Neuropsychologia, 49, 5, 1369-1372.

Carlesimo, G. A., Casadio, P., Sabbadini, M., Caltagirone, C. 1998. “Associative Visual Agnosia Resulting From A Disconnection Between Intact Visual Memory And Semantic Systems”, Cortex, 34, 563-576.

Chao, L. L., & Martin, A. 2000 “Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream”, NeuroImage, 12, 478-484.

Chemero, A. 2009. Radical Embodied Cognitive Science. MIT Press.

Coltheart, M. 1999. “Modularity and cognition”, Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 115-120.

Costantini, M., Ambrosini, E., Tieri, G., Sinigaglia, C., Committeri, G. 2010. “Where does an object trigger an action? An investigation about affordance in space”, Experimental Brain Reseach, 207, 95-103.

Fodor, J., & Pylyshyn, Z. 1981. “How Direct Is Visual Perception? Some Reflections on Gibson's Ecological Approach”, Cognition, 9, 139-196.

Freedberg, D. and Gallese, V. 2007 “Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience”. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11,197-203.

Gallagher, S. 2005. How the Body Shapes the Mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

Gallagher, S. 2008. “Direct Perception in the Intersubjective Context”, Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 535–543.

Gallese V., Ferrari P.F., Umiltà M.A. 2002. “The mirror matching system: a shared manifold for intersubjectivity” Behavioral Brain Sciences, 25, 35-36.

Gallese V., Sinigaglia C. 2011. “What is so special with Embodied Simulation” Trends Cognitive Science, 15, 11, 512-519.

Gallese, V., Murata, A., Kaseda, M., Niki, V., Sakata, T. 1994. “Deficit of hand preshaping after muscimol injection in monkey parietal cortex”, Neuroreport, 5,1525-1529.

Galletti, C., Gamberini, M., Kutz, D.F., Fattori, P., Luppino, G., Matelli, M. 2001. “The cortical connections of area V6A: an occipito-parietal network processing visual information”, The European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1, 572-1588.

Gamberini, M., Galletti, C., Luppino, G., Matelli, M. 2002. “Cytoarchitectonic organization of the functionally defined areas V6 and V6A in the parieto-occipital cortex of macaque brain”, Journal of Physiology, 543.

Gerrans, P. 2012. “Dream experience and a revisionist account of delusions of misidentification”, Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 217-227.

Gibson, J. J. 1979. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Goodale, M. A., Milner A. D. 1992. “Separate visual pathways for perception and action”, Trends Neurosci.15, 1, 20-25.

Grezes, J., Decety, J. 2002. “Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study”, Neuropsychologia, 40, 212–222.

Humphreys, G. W, Lamote, C., Lloyd-Jones, T. 1995. “An interactive activation approach to object processing: effects of structural similarity, name frequency and task in normality and pathology”, Memory,3, 535-586.

Jacob, P., Jeannerod, M. 2003. Ways of Seeing: The Scope and Limits of Visual Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

James, T. W., Culham, J., Humphrey, G, K, Milner, D., Melvyn, R. Goodale, A. 2003. “Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition but not object-directed grasping: an fMRI study”, Brain, 126, 2463-2475.

Lissauer, H.1890 “Ein fall von seelenblindheit nebst einem beitrage zur theorie derselben”, Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr, 21, 222-2270.

Marr, D. 1982. Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. New York: Freeman.

McCarthy, R. A., Worrington, E. K. 1986. “Visual associative agnosia: a clinico-anatomical study of a single case”, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 49, 1233-1240.

Mesulam, M. M. 1985. Patterns in behavioral Neuroanatomy. In M. M. Mesulam ed. Principles of Behavioral Neurology. Philadelphia: Davis. 1-70.

Michaels, C.F., Carello, C. 1981. Direct Perception. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentecie Hall.

Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. 1995. The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Milner, A.D., Perrett, D.I., Johnston, R.S., Benson, P.J., Jordan, T.R., Heeley, D.W. 1991. “Perception and Action in Visual form Agnosia”, Brain,114, 405-428.

Murata A, Gallese V, Luppino G, Kaseda M, Sakata, H. 2000. “Selectivity for the shape, size and orientation of objects in the hand-manipulation-related neurons in the anterior intraparietal AIP area of the macaque” Journal of Neurophysiology, 83, 2580-2601.

Murata, A., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Raos, V., & Rizzolatti, G. 1997. “Object Representation in the Ventral Premotor Cortex Area F5 of the Monkey”, Journal of Neurophysiology, 78, 2226-2230.

Nanay, B. 2013. Between Perception and Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Noë, A. 2005. Action in Perception. MIT Press.

Norman, J. 2002. “Two visual systems and two theories of perception: An attempt to reconcile the constructivist and ecological approaches”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 73-144.

Palmer, S.E. 1999. Vision Science: Photons to Phenomenology. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.

Petit, S. L., Pegna, A. J., Harris, M. I. 2006. “Automatic motor cortex activation for natural as compared to awkward grips of a manipulable object”, Experimental Brain Research, 168, 120-130.

Pylyshyn, Z. 1986. Computation and cognition: Toward a foundation for cognitive science. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Riddoch, J. M., Humphrey, W. G. 2003. “Visual Agnosia”. Neurol Clin Am 21, 501-520.

Riddoch, M. J. , Humphreys, G. W. 1987. “Visual object processing in optic aphasia: a case of semantic access agnosia”, Cognitive Neuropsychology,4, 131-185.

Rizzolatti, G., Camarda, R., Fogassi, L., Gentilucci, M., Luppino, G., Matelli, M. 1988. “Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements”, Experimental Brain Research, 71, 491-507.

Rizzolatti, G., Luppino, G. 2001. “The cortical motor system”, Neuron, 31, 889–901.

Rizzolatti, G., Matelli, M. 2003. “Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions”, Experimental Brain Research, 153,146-157.

Sakata, H., Taira, M., Murata, A., Mine, S. 1995. “Neural mechanisms of visual guidance of hand action in the parietal cortex of the monkey”, Cerebral Cortex, 5, 429-438.

Schenk, T. 2010. “Visuomotor robustness is based on integration not segregation”, Visual Research, 50, 2627-2632.

Schenk, T. 2012. “No Dissociation between Perception and Action in Patient DF When Haptic Feedback is Withdrawn”, The Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 6, 2013-2017.

Shelton, P. A., Bowers, D., Duara, R., Heillman, L. K. 1994. “Apperceptive Visual Agnosia: A Case Study”, Brain and Cognition, 25, 1-23.

Shikata, E., Hamzei, F., Glauche, V., Koch, M., Weiller, C., Binkofski, F., Büchel, C. 2003. “Functional Properties and Interaction of the Anterior and Posterior Intraparietal Areas in Humans”, European Journal of Neuoroscience, 17, 1105-1110.

Srivatasva, S, Orban, G. A., De Maziére, P. A., Jenssen, P. 2009. “A Distinct Representation of Three-Dimensional Shape in Macaque Anterior Intraparietal Area: Fast, Metric, and Coarse”, The Journal of Neuroscience, 2934, 10613-10626.

Ullman, S. 1980. “Against Direct Perception”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 373-415.

Vecera, S. P., Gilds, K.S. 1998. “What processing is impaired in apperceptive agnosia? Evidence from normal subjects”, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 568-580.

Verhoef, B.E. Vogels, R., Janssen, P. 2010. “Contribution of inferior temporal and posterior parietal activity to three-dimensional shape perception”, Current Biology, 20, 909-913.

Von Helmholtz, H. 1867. Handbuch der physiologischen Optik, Leipzig: Leopold Voss.

Warrington, EK, Shallice, T. 1984. “Category-specific semantic impairments”, Brain,107, 829-854.

Westwood, D. A., & Goodale, M. A. 2003. “Perceptual illusion and the real-time control of action”, Spatial Vision, 163/4, 243–254.

Withagen, R., Chemero, A. 2011. “Affordances and classification: On the significance of a sidebar in James Gibson's last book”. Philosophical Psychology, 25, 4, 521-537.

Zeki, S. 1993. A Vision of the Brain. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific.

Share

COinS